Finishing up the discussion of Kahneman’s Thinking Fast and Slow on a
cautionary note, the book’s model of a human mind broken into two parts: a
fast, associative System 1 that does the bulk of our day-to-day cognitive work
and a slower, deliberative System 2 that analyzes deeply, handles counter-intuitive
situations, and takes control when necessary should be seen for what it is: a
metaphor.
The author himself announces the metaphorical nature of his construct
of the mind, telling us early on that his creation of the labels System 1 and
System 2 was a deliberate artifice which, among other things, provides his
alleged System 1 some named characters for one of the stories it uses to
achieve understanding. And as much as the
metaphor of slow and fast mental entities
working in partnership helps explain a wide number of observable phenomena
(such as our tendency to fall for visual and cognitive illusions), we shouldn’t
lose sight of the fact that Kahneman’s is just one of a long line of metaphors
used to describe human behavior.
For example, while it has become fashionable to sneer at Freud, his
identification of a conscious and unconscious mind (not to mention the Ego,
Superhero and Id he posited) not only impacted both the scientific and wider
culture, it also served as the foundation for modern public relations and advertising
when put to practical use by Freud’s nephew Edward Bernays. And our culture’s fondness for the notion of
a Left-Right creative-logical split in our brain (despite an absence of
physical evidence for such a divide) simply proves how much we humans desire to
break our world into neatly labeled categories.
Now it may very well turn out that Kahneman’s fast/slow dichotomy will
withstand experimental scrutiny over time.
But work done since then (including a colleague’s thesis I’m currently
reading that proposes three different modes of thinking vs. Kahneman’s measly
two) is already building on and refining the original hypothesis. So before we start abandoning previous
theories and models and start thinking of ourselves entirely in fast and slow
terms, it’s best to exercise some humility and accept the fact that this model,
while useful, may very well turn out to be wrong.
Shifting gears to political matters (which is supposed to be the
subject of this blog), I must also admit to becoming uncomfortable when the
author began suggesting examples of his theories could be put to practical
advantage.
If our minds work a certain way, the author asserts, why not take
advantage of this phenomenon to gently move (or nudge) people towards socially
acceptable or preferable behavior?
The classic example of this idea comes out of the near 100% volunteer
rate for organ donation in certain European countries, vs. a far smaller rate
for Americans. This difference turns out
not to be the result of differing levels of generosity between cultures, but rather
a different in forms – specifically organ-donation volunteer forms which are opt-out
abroad (meaning volunteering for organ donation is the default choice) but
opt-in here in the US. So if telling
your printer or web page designer to change which box is checked automatically
can lead to such a huge increase in some socially beneficial good, why not
apply it to other societal problems (such as improving American saving habits
by making joining an automatic savings plan the default option)?
The trouble is that you pretty rapidly run out of examples of causes where
playing on global cognitive hard wiring will lead to unquestionably good
outcomes. Kahneman highlights the popularity
of Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein’s recent book Nudge which shows how slight
manipulations in how information is presented can gently push people towards
making the right choices about their money, health and overall happiness. And he’s particularly excited that one of the
book’s authors is playing a role in the current US administration, providing an
avenue to put these ideas into practice.
But who gets to decide what behaviors the population will be nudged into? And who gets to determine where friendly
nudging ends and outright manipulation begins? Historically, powerful
persuasive techniques such as classical rhetoric or Freudian science have been
embraced by politicians and advertisers who want to get us to do what they
want. So what is to prevent new
techniques based on what we think we know about our fast and slow processors
from simply becoming the friendliest option on a list that also includes propaganda
and coercion?
It may be a fool’s errand, but if this blog is about anything it’s
about trying to engage that reasoning part of our minds (or our slow processor
if you like) much more than often we do when it comes time to make key
decisions that can affect our future as individuals and as a country. And while it would be far simpler if we could
all be made to “do the right thing” by simply switching around some wording or
some options on a form, fears over who gets to be the nudger makes the far
messier option of trying to get people to think for themselves a preferable
alternative.